konstrukt ADHD u dorosłych.pdf

(77 KB) Pobierz
92861837 UNPDF
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
0021-843X/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.519
Axis I and II Comorbidity in Adults With ADHD
Torri W. Miller and Joel T. Nigg
Michigan State University
Stephen V. Faraone
State University of New York Upstate Medical University
Ongoing debate over the validity of the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) construct in
adulthood is fueled in part by uncertainty regarding implications of potentially extensive yet incompletely
described comorbid Axis I and II psychopathology. Three hundred sixty-three adults ages 18 to 37
completed semistructured clinical interviews; informants were also interviewed, and best estimate
diagnoses were obtained. Results were as follows: First, ADHD combined type (ADHD–C) had an
excess of externalizing and internalizing Axis I disorders, suggesting a gradient-of-severity relationship
between it and ADHD inattentive type (ADHD–I). Second, ADHD–C and ADHD–I did not differ in
frequency of Axis II disorders. Third, however, ADHD overall was associated with increased rates of
Axis II disorders, compared with rates in non-ADHD control participants, including both Cluster B
(primarily borderline personality disorder) and Cluster C disorders. Fourth, ADHD incrementally
accounted for clinician-rated global assessment of functioning scores above and beyond comorbid
conditions or symptoms on either Axis I or Axis II. Results further inform nosology of ADHD in adults.
Keywords: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), comorbidity, DSM–IV subtypes, personality
disorders, functional impairment
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is character-
ized by a persistent pattern of inattentive, hyperactive, and impul-
sive behaviors that begin in early childhood, often persist through-
out development, and interfere with adaptive functioning
(American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Population surveys es-
timate the prevalence of ADHD in adulthood to be about 5%
(Faraone & Biederman, 2005; Kessler et al., 2006), and neurobio-
logic and genetic findings from adults with ADHD are similar to
results seen in children (Faraone, 2004). Although interest in
ADHD in adults has been long-standing (Wender, 1974) and has
intensified in recent years (Faraone, 2000), adult ADHD remains
relatively underinvestigated. Historically, ADHD was primarily
conceptualized as a disorder of childhood. Subsequently, long-
term follow-up studies of children with ADHD established that
ADHD persists into adulthood in a substantial proportion of cases
(Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2004; Mannuzza, Klein,
& Moulton, 2003; Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). A meta-analysis of
follow-up studies found that, although estimates of persistence
vary with how the diagnosis is defined, 65% of children with
ADHD will show impairing symptoms of ADHD in adulthood
(Faraone, Biederman, & Mick, 2006).
Patterns of clinical comorbidity, both Axis I and Axis II, are
critical to evaluating the clinical validity of ADHD in adults for
multiple reasons. First, comorbidity is likely very common. This is
the case in children with ADHD (Biederman et al., 1993) and
likely to also be true in adults. Although description of Axis I
comorbidity has begun in adult ADHD (Barkley, 2002; Barkley,
Fischer, Fletcher, & Smallish, 2002; Barkley et al., 2004; Marks,
Newcorn, & Halperin, 2001; Murphy & Barkley, 1996; Young,
Toone, & Tyson, 2003), Axis II comorbidity is inadequately
mapped as we describe later. Even for Axis I psychopathology,
findings in adults with ADHD have been somewhat inconsistent,
perhaps because of relatively small sample sizes in many studies;
predominantly male samples; and in some studies, reliance on
rating scales or self-report to assess ADHD. With regard to gender,
results of a prospective follow-up study of girls with ADHD
showed continued impairment into adolescence but found only
limited differences between ADHD subtypes (Hinshaw, Owens,
Sami, & Fargeon, 2006).
Second, developmental change was not addressed in the Diag-
nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM–
IV ; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria, which are the
same regardless of age. Yet substantial developmental changes
occur in attention and activity level from childhood to adulthood
(Faraone et al., 2006). For example, hyperactive behaviors appar-
ently decline with development relative to inattentive symptoms
(Hart, Lahey, Loeber, & Hanson, 1994). Adults with ADHD
consequently may experience relatively more impairment from
inattention, disorganization, and subjective restlessness rather than
hyperactivity. Consequently, the validity and appropriateness of
the DSM–IV ADHD subtype structure (combined, ADHD–C; pri-
marily inattentive, ADHD–I; primarily hyperactive–impulsive,
ADHD–H) for clinical characterization of adults is unclear. As a
result, clinical comorbidity, which tends to differ in ADHD sub-
types in children, may not show similar patterns in adults, at least
when DSM–IV criteria are used.
In children, clinical data support the validity of distinguishing
ADHD–I from ADHD–C (Carlson & Mann, 2000). ADHD–C is
associated with more externalizing problems (Eiraldi, Power, &
Nezu, 1997; Gaub & Carlson, 1997), whereas at least some studies
Torri W. Miller and Joel T. Nigg, Department of Psychology, Michigan
State University; Stephen V. Faraone, Departments of Psychiatry and
Neuroscience and Physiology, State University of New York Upstate
Medical University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Torri W.
Miller, Department of Psychology, Michigan State University, 43 Psychol-
ogy Building, East Lansing, MI 48824. E-mail: torri@msu.edu
519
2007, Vol. 116, No. 3, 519–528
Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association
520
MILLER, NIGG, AND FARAONE
suggest either that ADHD–I is associated with relatively more
internalizing disorders or problems (Lahey et al., 1994; Weiss,
Worling, & Wasdell, 2003) or that there are equivalent levels of
internalizing in the two subtypes (Eiraldi, Power, Karustis, &
Goldstein, 2000). Therefore, a strong prediction from the hypoth-
esis that ADHD–C and ADHD–I are distinct disorders (Milich,
Balentine, & Lynam, 2001) would be that in adults, ADHD–C
would be associated with an excess of externalizing disorders,
whereas ADHD–I would be associated with an excess of internal-
izing disorders. However, if ADHD–I is hypothesized to be a mild
form of the more severe ADHD–C (Faraone, Biederman, Weber,
& Russell, 1998), then it would be predicted that by adulthood,
ADHD–C would show excess comorbidity across all domains
relative to ADHD–I.
Only a handful of studies have examined this question in ADHD
subtypes in adults, with mixed results. Millstein, Wilens, Bieder-
man, and Spencer (1997) found more bipolar, oppositional, and
substance use disorders in ADHD–C than ADHD–I or ADHD–H.
Murphy, Barkley, and Bush (2002) found that both ADHD–C and
ADHD–I had excess dysthymia, alcohol and drug dependence–
abuse, learning disorders, and psychological distress, but
ADHD–C was further associated with oppositional defiant disor-
der (ODD), suicide attempts, arrests, and interpersonal hostility
and paranoia. In a study of children and adolescents, Volk, Hen-
derson, Neuman, and Todd (2006) found that impairment among
ADHD subtypes was not increased by comorbidity with conduct
disorder (CD), ODD, or major depressive disorder.
Third, aside from antisocial personality disorder, which is often
associated with ADHD in adulthood (Biederman et al., 1993;
Downey, Stelson, Pomerleau, & Giordani, 1997; Faraone et al.,
2000; Levin, Evans, & Kleber, 1998; Loeber, Burke, & Lahey,
2002), surprisingly little research has considered Axis II comor-
bidity. None, to our knowledge, have considered whether Axis II
comorbidity can account for ADHD-related impairment. Data on
normal personality traits suggest that ADHD may be associated
with extreme standing on personality (Nigg et al., 2002). Such
findings, as well as the aforementioned stability and chronicity of
the impulsive and dysregulated behaviors that compose ADHD,
suggest a theoretical connection between ADHD symptoms and
personality traits and, by extension, personality disorders —which
are defined as chronic, maladaptive personality traits (American
Psychiatric Association, 2000). This connection emerges in vari-
ous ways. One possibility is that ADHD alters personality and thus
increases risk for personality disorder later in development. An-
other possibility is that both ADHD and certain personality disor-
ders are related to the same extreme personality diathesis. In either
case, the question arises whether both ADHD and personality
disorder diagnoses add incremental validity in predicting impair-
ment.
Cluster B personality disorders are conceptually the most sim-
ilar to ADHD (Rey, Morris-Yates, Singh, Andrews, & Stewart,
1995) and therefore are the group we expected to be most likely to
co-occur with adult ADHD. They are characterized by inability to
control or regulate behavior, affect, and cognition and by social
and interpersonal problems that are at least superficially similar to
those seen in ADHD (Akiskal et al., 1985; Tzelepis, Schubiner, &
Warbasse, 1995; Weiss, Hechtman, & Weiss, 1999). Indeed, the
idea that ADHD may be a precursor to borderline personality
disorder (BPD) has been long-standing (Akiskal et al., 1985),
although research on this overlap is scarce. Yet a small number of
studies suggest that ADHD is associated with BPD (Dowson et al.,
2004; Rey et al., 1995), may be superimposed on the personality
difficulties of those patients with BPD (Weiss et al., 1999), and
appears more often in the childhood history of patients with adult
BPD (Fossati, Novella, Donati, Donini, & Maffei, 2002). How-
ever, these studies were relatively small and/or did not use
DSM–IV criteria or structured diagnostic interviews, leaving their
conclusions in some doubt. Other personality traits and disorders
that may be related to adult ADHD include histrionic and narcis-
sistic traits (Fischer, Barkley, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2002; May &
Bos, 2000), obsessive–compulsive personality disorder (OCPD),
and histrionic personality disorder (Modestin, Matutat, &
Wuermle, 2001), as well as personality dimensions such as novelty
or sensation seeking (Downey et al., 1997).
Fourth, the extensive comorbidity associated with ADHD un-
derscores divergent conceptualizations of how ADHD is taxonom-
ically related to comorbid conditions (Faraone, 2000). Within a
hierarchical framework, a patient meeting criteria for two or more
disorders would only be diagnosed with the higher ranking disor-
der; DSM–IV follows this approach to some degree for ADHD by
requiring that the ADHD symptoms not be better accounted for by
some other mental disorder. The comorbidity framework allows
for the assessment of all disorders; diagnostic overlap is viewed
more as the rule than the exception. From a hierarchical perspec-
tive, it remains unclear whether there is additional clinical utility in
diagnosing ADHD in the presence of co-occurring disorders or
their symptoms. One way to assess this is to examine whether
ADHD adds to the statistical prediction of impairment after co-
morbid disorders have been covaried. Lahey et al. (2004) found
this relationship in children: Children with ADHD continued to
display significant impairment relative to control participants
when comorbid psychopathology was statistically controlled. This
critical question of ADHD’s incremental validity relative to clin-
ical impairment remains essentially untested in adults, although
some data show that ADHD is a risk factor for substance use
disorders in adults, even after the researchers controlled for a
history of conduct or bipolar disorders (Biederman et al., 1995). In
a controlled study of community adults, ADHD (without regard to
comorbid conditions) was associated with impairment in terms of
lower degree attainment, less employment, more job changes,
more arrests and divorce, and lower personal satisfaction (Bieder-
man et al., 2006), but comorbid conditions were not controlled.
Aims of the Present Study
The present study sought to clarify clinical validity of the
ADHD construct in adults by examining independent predictors of
adaptive impairment and subtype differences in relation to Axis I
and Axis II comorbidity. Key questions were (a) whether
ADHD–C accrues more comorbid externalizing disorders than
ADHD–I and controls and whether the comorbid profile differs
across ADHD subtypes (interaction of Comorbid Domain
Sub-
type), (b) which domains of Axis II personality disorders are
associated with ADHD regardless of subtype, and (c) whether
ADHD or its symptoms have an influence on impairment above
and beyond that which is accounted for by Axis I and Axis II
psychopathology.
COMORBIDITY IN ADULT ADHD
521
Method
a retrospective K–SADS ADHD module. The second informant,
who knew the participant currently (usually a spouse or friend),
completed the Conners peer rating, the Barkley and Murphy peer
ratings on adult symptoms, a brief screen of antisocial behavior
and drug and alcohol use, and a structured interview about the
participant’s current ADHD symptoms, using the modified
K–SADS for current symptoms.
Establishment of best estimate diagnosis for ADHD. A diag-
nostic team that included a licensed clinical social worker, a
licensed clinical psychologist, and a board certified psychiatrist
then arrived at a “best estimate” diagnosis (Faraone, 2000). Each
team member reviewed all available information from the semi-
structured interviews, and rating scales to arrive at a judgment
about ADHD present or absent, ADHD subtype, and comorbid
disorders. In the case of disagreement, consensus was reached by
discussion. Interrater agreement on presence or absence of ADHD
(any type) was satisfactory ( k .80), and agreement on ADHD
subtype (combined, inattentive, or hyperactive) in childhood and
adulthood was also adequate (ranging from k .74 to .85). In view
of disagreement about whether the subtype classification should be
represented by childhood or adult symptom profiles (Faraone,
2000), we relied on the adult subtype here.
Assessment of comorbid Axis I disorders. The Structured Clin-
ical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders (SCID–I; First,
Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997) was administered by a trained
master’s level clinician after extensive training in the interview
and checkout of taped interviews for validity by First and col-
leagues at Columbia. Diagnoses examined in the current study
included major depressive episode, dysthymic disorder, bipolar
disorder, substance abuse and dependence, psychotic disorders,
obsessive–compulsive, panic disorder, agoraphobia, simple pho-
bia, social phobia, and eating disorders. Autistic disorder was
screened by added symptom questions and was a rule out. Twenty-
five SCID interviews were videotaped and coded by two qualified
interviewers to assess reliability of the interview procedures. Re-
liability for comorbid disorders was acceptable (e.g., substance use
disorder, k .83; mood disorder, k .80; anxiety disorder, k
.71; antisocial personality disorder, k .84).
Assessment of Axis II disorders. Participants completed the
SCID–II prescreening form. Any disorder for which at least one
symptom was endorsed was followed up with the SCID–II inter-
view module for that disorder. This procedure results in a very low
rate of false negatives (participants who endorse zero symptoms on
the questionnaire virtually never have a disorder on administration
of the full SCID–II interview) while capturing potential personal-
ity disorders and assessing them after the screen (First, Gibbon,
Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997).
Participants
Overview. The sample included 363 adults (185 men and 178
women) ages 18 to 37 years. Participants’ race closely mirrored the
surrounding community from which they were obtained, with 86%
Caucasian. Following procedures described later, participants were
grouped into ADHD (any subtype; n
152) and non-ADHD
211). For some analyses, participants
with ADHD were further divided into ADHD–I ( n 69) and
ADHD–C ( n 64). We had only 19 participants with ADHD–H;
this group was judged too small for reliable analysis and so was
omitted from subtype analyses.
Multistage recruitment process. Participants were recruited by
a broad net of public advertisements, including radio, newspaper,
movie theaters, and mailings to local clinics, in an effort to obtain
as broadly representative a volunteer sample as possible. We
advertised for ADHD with separate ads targeted at “individuals
that have been diagnosed or suspect they have an attention deficit
disorder (ADHD or ADD), or other attention problems.” We
advertised for the non-ADHD comparison group with ads seeking
“volunteers in good health who do not have attention problems.”
These efforts resulted in 623 applicants contacting the project
office. These prospective participants underwent a phone screen-
ing to check rule outs (inclusionary criteria were ages 18–40, no
sensorimotor disability, no neurological illness, native English
speaking, and currently prescribed antidepressant, antipsychotic,
or anticonvulsant medications). At this stage, 533 participants
were coded as eligible and went on to Stage 2. At Stage 2, eligible
participants completed semistructured clinical interviews and nor-
mative rating scales to assess ADHD and comorbid Axis I and
Axis II disorders as detailed next. Those data were then reviewed
by the best estimate clinical team to determine final diagnostic
assignment and study eligibility. One hundred seventy participants
were excluded at this stage (because of lack of cross-informant
convergence enabling clear classification as ADHD or non-
ADHD, current major depression, current severe substance use,
psychosis, or brain–head injury), yielding the final N 363.
Diagnostic instruments. A retrospective Kiddie Schedule for
Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K–SADS; Puig-Antich &
Ryan, 1986) was administered to assess ADHD. This well-
established procedure (Biederman et al., 1992; Biederman, Fara-
one, Keenan, Knee, & Tsuang, 1990) included the diagnoses of
childhood DSM–IV ADHD, CD, and ODD. Current symptoms
were assessed by structured interview and included K–SADS
ADHD questions worded appropriately for current adult symptoms
(Biederman et al., 1992). Respondents also completed the Barkley
and Murphy (1998) Current ADHD Symptoms rating scale. We
obtained normative, standardized dimensional ratings of attention
problems as well as other current symptoms by having participants
complete the Conners, Erhardt, and Sparrow (1999) Adult ADHD
Rating Scale, the Achenbach (1991) Young Adult Self Report
scale, and the Brown (1996) Adult ADHD rating scale.
To address potential reporting bias in self-report interviews of
ADHD (Barkley et al., 2002), two informants who knew the
participant well were interviewed. One informant, who knew the
participant as a child (usually a parent) reported on the target
participant’s childhood behaviors via an ADHD Rating Scale and
Assessment of Impairment
Global assessment of functioning (GAF; American Psychiatric
Association, 2000) scores were assigned by the interviewing cli-
nician at the end of the structured clinical interviews. This score of
overall functional adjustment was used as an index of impairment;
high scores indicate better functioning and low scores indicate
more impairment. To evaluate reliability of the impairment scores,
20 SCID–KSAD interviews were taped and reviewed by a second
clinical rater, blind to the GAF rating or diagnoses of the first rater.
GAF scores were assigned by the second rater and were compared
control participants ( n
522
MILLER, NIGG, AND FARAONE
with those assigned by the first rater. The interclass correlation
(absolute agreement) of .714 reflected adequate interrater reliabil-
ity.
disorders (all mood and anxiety disorders). Because these were
ordinal count variables, we analyzed group effects with multino-
mial logistic regression. To obtain adequate cell sizes, we catego-
rized number of externalizing and internalizing disorders as fol-
lows: 0 (none), 1, and 2 or more. Table 2 shows the resultant
frequencies. We checked all models for Sex Group interactions
and they were not significant, so we simply covaried sex.
For externalizing disorders, the three-group multinomial logistic
regression model (with sex covaried) indicated a significant model
overall,
Data Analytic Plan
We tested a series of questions regarding subtype effects and
Axis I disorders with multinomial and binomial logistic regression,
with sex effects covaried. We tested a series of questions concern-
ing impairment using linear multiple regression and multiple lo-
gistic regression models. Statistical power for all analyses ex-
ceeded .90 to detect Cohen’s (1992) medium-sized effects ( r
.15), with the exception of some pairwise post hoc tests.
2
(6, N 344) 27.1, p .001, –2LL 54.3; the main
effect of group was significant as well,
(4, N 344) 20.8, p
.001, –2LL 75.0. Using the control group as the reference,
presence of one externalizing disorder was more likely for partic-
ipants with ADHD–C (odds ratio [OR] 2.05, p .050), but not
for ADHD–I ( p .766). Presence of two or more externalizing
disorders was more likely for ADHD–C (OR 5.12, p .001)
and ADHD–I (OR 2.12, p .038). Thus, ADHD–C conferred
a fivefold increase in risk for two or more externalizing disorders
whereas ADHD–I conferred a doubling of such risk versus the
control group. The OR was significantly higher for ADHD–C than
ADHD–I for two or more disorders ( p .044), indicating that
ADHD–C conferred more risk of externalizing disorder than
ADHD–I.
For internalizing disorders, the three-group multinomial logistic
regression model (with sex covaried) indicated a significant model
overall,
2
Results
Sample Description and Review of Potential Demographic
Confounds
Demographic information for the sample is provided in Table 1.
Ratings data all showed marked clinical elevations in the ADHD
sample, indicating validity of ADHD assignments regardless of
instrument or model used. Parental household incomes were sim-
ilar in the two groups ( p .4), indicating that they came from
similar socioeconomic backgrounds. Despite this, and consistent
with prior reports (Murphy & Barkley, 1996), individuals with
ADHD were less likely to complete high school than control
participants (see Table 1). The ADHD group was less likely to
attend college than the control group (53% vs. 62%, p .05).
Those who attended a 4-year college were more likely to be
control participants than participants with ADHD (36% vs. 20%,
p .01), whereas those who attended a 2-year community college
were more likely to have ADHD than be control participants (18%
vs. 10%, p .01). Thus, the ADHD group had lower educational
attainment overall. Personal incomes were qualitatively lower for
the nonstudent ADHD ( M $32,000) than non-ADHD group
( M 40,200) though this effect was not significant. The gender
difference, with a greater proportion of male participants with
ADHD (see Table 1), occurred despite our efforts to overselect
female participants with ADHD; it is common in studies of ADHD
and in part may reflect the male preponderance of ADHD in the
population. Because some of the comorbid disorders vary by
gender, we controlled statistically for gender. Groups did not differ
significantly in percentage of minority participants, although there
were qualitatively more minorities in the ADHD group ( p .07).
We therefore checked all results with ethnicity covaried; results
were unchanged from those reported in this article. Consistent with
other studies of ADHD, participants with any ADHD were more
likely to have substance use disorder,
2
(6, N 344) 39.7, p .001, –2LL 49.6; the main
effect of group was again significant,
(4, N 344) 33.3, p
.001, –2LL 82.9. Using the control group as the reference, we
found that presence of one internalizing disorder was more likely
for ADHD–C (OR 4.59, p .001) and ADHD–I (OR 3.22,
p .001). Presence of two or more internalizing disorders was
significant for ADHD–C (OR 3.76, p .001) and for ADHD–I
(OR 4.02, p .001). Although the OR was slightly higher for
ADHD–I than ADHD–C for two or more disorders, the ADHD–C
versus ADHD–I effect was nonsignificant ( p .854). As can be
seen in Table 2, although the ADHD–I group had slightly more
likelihood of two or more internalizing disorders, the ADHD–C
group had more likelihood of internalizing disorder overall, al-
though this difference also was nonsignificant.
In summary, ADHD–C was associated with more comorbid
externalizing disorders than ADHD–I or non-ADHD status. How-
ever, contrary to a “distinct disorder” hypothesis, as displayed in
Table 2 ADHD–I was associated with qualitatively lower, not
higher, rates of total internalizing disorders than ADHD–C, al-
though ADHD–I was associated with a slightly but not signifi-
cantly greater chance of having two or more internalizing disorders
versus ADHD–C.
2
2
(1, N 363) 9.22, p
.01; mood disorder,
2
(1, N 363) 23.70, p .001; anxiety
Hypothesis 2: Axis II Comorbidity in Relation to ADHD
2
(1, N 363) 8.81, p .01; and antisocial person-
ality disorder,
2
(1, N 363) 7.32, p .01, than the non-
ADHD comparison group.
For these analyses, we created three personality disorder com-
posite variables: Cluster A disorders present or absent (presence of
one or more of paranoid, schizoid, and/or schizotypal personality
disorder), Cluster B disorders present or absent (presence of one or
more of borderline, antisocial, histrionic, and/or narcissistic per-
sonality disorder), and Cluster C disorders present or absent (pres-
ence of one or more of avoidant, dependent, and/or OCPD). Table
3 shows the frequencies of the Axis II disorders by cluster for each
group. The ADHD subtypes did not differ on any of these clusters:
Hypothesis 1: Subtype Comorbid Profiles for Axis I
Disorders
To test this question, we summed total number of lifetime (a)
externalizing disorders (lifetime ODD, CD, substance use disor-
ders, and antisocial personality disorder), and (b) internalizing
disorder,
Table 1
Description of Sample
Control
( n 211)
Any ADHD
( n 152)
ADHD–I
( n 69)
ADHD–C
( n 64)
Variable
n % MSDn % MSD n % MSDn % MSD
p
p
Male
89 42
96 63
.001 47 68
41 64
.504
White
174 83
136 90
.062 60 87
58 91
.625
Age (in years)
23.7
4.5
23.7
4.6 1.00
23.31 4.7
23.78 4.8
.828
Conners ADHD T score
47.2 11.1
62.1 10.1
.001
58.51 9.7
66.10 9.5
.526
No. of current DSM inattentive symptoms
1.81 2.2
7.0
1.7
.001
7.26 1.5
7.29 1.4
.922
No. of current DSM hyperactivity–impulsivity
symptoms
1.7
2.0
5.68 2.5
.001
3.74 2.1
7.10 1.5
.001
No. of childhood DSM inattentive symptoms
1.93 2.1
6.78 1.6
.001
6.99 1.3
7.06 1.4
.188
No. of childhood DSM hyperactivity–impulsivity
symptoms
1.64 1.7
5.13 2.2
.001
3.63 1.9
6.23 1.7
.001
Parent annual combined income a
3.73 1.1
3.64 1.2
.484
3.83 1.1
3.43 1.2
.05
Lifetime substance use disorder
84 40
88 57
.001 35 50
44 67
.112
Lifetime mood disorder
58 28
82 53
.001 36 51
38 58
.370
Lifetime any anxiety disorder
36 17
47 30
.05 24 34
18 27
.888
Lifetime antisocial personality disorder
8 4
15 11
.05
6 10
8 15
.356
Completed high school
137 94
91 84
.05 41 87
38 79
.298
Marital status
.865
.500
Married
26 14
16 13
6 10
7 14
Single
155 81
103 84
50 86
43 84
Divorced
9 5
4 3
2 4
1 2
Note. The probability value reflects the two-group comparison (control vs. any ADHD) and is based on an independent-samples t test for continuous variables or a chi-square test for dichotomous
variables. Sample sizes varied slightly for some measures because of missing data for some disorders. ADHD attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ADHD–I ADHD inattentive type;
ADHD–C ADHD combined type.
a Parent annual combined income categories were as follows: 1 $25,000–$50,000; 2 $50,000–$75,000; 3 $75,000–$100,000; 4 greater than $100,000.
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin