3. Internal Reconstruction.pdf

(222 KB) Pobierz
3. Internal Reconstruction : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
3. Internal Reconstruction : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:29 PM
3. Internal Reconstruction
DON RINGE
Subject
Linguistics » Historical Linguistics
DOI:
10.1111/b.9781405127479.2004.00005.x
“Internal reconstruction” (IR) is the exploitation of patterns in the synchronic grammar of a single language
or dialect to recover information about its prehistory. The methods of IR are generally less reliable than the
standard methods of comparative reconstruction (CR; see Rankin, this volume) for the following reasons.
Many of the changes that occur naturally in languages over time eliminate language structures in
unrecoverable ways. These include the replacement of lexemes by completely different words (e.g., the
replacement of Old English (OE) sinwealt by Middle English round ); the syntactic merger or loss of
grammatical categories (e.g., the merger of the dative and instrumental cases within the OE period, and the
subsequent loss of the dative); the leveling of morpho-phonemic alternations (on which see further below);
the unconditioned merger or loss of phonemes; and other, less common processes (see Hoenigswald 1960:
28–37, 90–1). CR circumvents the effects of these changes by adducing evidence from related languages or
dialects in which the same changes have not occurred; IR has no comparably straightforward means of
“undoing” the changes. In the absence of comparative evidence, IR must make use of several assumptions
about which types of changes are most likely to have given rise to the synchronic patterns observed. Many of
those assumptions are not problematic, but the only one that is completely reliable in every case is the
fundamental observation on which CR is also based – namely, that sound change is overwhelmingly regular.
IR is therefore of limited use in historical linguistics; CR is so much more reliable that it is preferred
whenever possible. But there are situations in which the linguist is not offered a choice, either because a
language is not demon-strably related to any other, or because it has been developing in isolation from its
nearest kin for so long that comparative work encounters massive practical difficulties. A firm grasp of the
principles of IR is therefore an essential part of the historical linguist's professional knowledge.
Like all methods of linguistic reconstruction, IR proceeds by making inferences about unobservable stages of
a language's development on the basis of what is known from the observed history of languages. Therefore
one can best gain an understanding of IR by studying, in light of the known principles of language change,
linguistic patterns whose origin and development is already well understood. Most of this chapter will
accordingly be devoted to discussion of relevant examples. Since the standard theoretical treatment of
Hoenigswald (1960: 68–9, 99–111) can scarcely be bettered, I will concentrate on the practical problems
that IR involves.
The structural patterns that are most useful for IR are alternations between (lexical) phonemes in
morphological contexts. I shall first discuss and exemplify the exploitation of individual alternations, then
consider other types of patterns that can be used in IR. For further discussion see now Fox (1995: 145–
216). 1
1 Alternations Resulting from Conditioned Merger
IR most often exploits alternations resulting from the conditioned merger of phonemes, which is necessarily
accompanied by split of one of the original phonemes (Hoenigswald 1960: 91–3); in Hoenigswald's
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g97814051274795
Page 1 of 14
665559040.007.png 665559040.008.png
3. Internal Reconstruction : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:29 PM
accompanied by split of one of the original phonemes (Hoenigswald 1960: 91–3); in Hoenigswald's
maximally concise formulation:
phonemic SPLIT in several of its varieties leads to morphophonemic alternation, provided that
morph boundaries fall between the conditioning and the conditioned phoneme or phonemes
and provided that the same phoneme in the same morph thus comes within the range
sometimes of one, sometimes of the other, type of conditioning phoneme or phonemes.
(p. 100)
The type of conditioned merger that presents us with patterns of data most favorable to IR involves the
neutralization of phonemic contrasts in easily recognized environments which occur often enough to provide
numerous examples (cf. Hoenigswald 1960: 100–2). A straightforward case is the devoicing of word-final
obstruents observable in (Standard) German. 2 Especially numerous are examples involving stem-final
alveolar stops, of which the following partial noun paradigms are typical: 3
Singular Plural
Meaning
/ta:t/
/ta:tәn/
‘deed’
/p f a:t/ /p f a:dә/
‘path’
/gra:t/ /gra:dә/
‘degree, rank’
/gra:t/ /gra:tә/
‘edge, ridge’
/špa:t/ /špa:tә/ d /
špe:tә/
‘spar' [mineral]
/ra:t/
/re:tә/
‘council, councilor’
/ra:t/
/re:dәr/
‘wheeL’
It is clear that the shape of the plural cannot be predicted from the shape of the singular; and one of the
unpredictable details is whether the final /t/ of the singular reappears in the plural or /d/ appears in its
place. The same phenomenon occurs in the inflection of other classes of words which have endingless forms.
For example, among adjectives one finds /bunt/ ‘mottled' with inflected forms /buntә/, etc., but /gәzunt/
‘healthy,’ /gәzundә/, etc.; among verbs one finds narrative preterite /zi: ri:t/ ‘she advised' and /zi: ri:tәn/
‘they advised,’ but /zi: fermi:t/ ‘she avoided' and /zi: fermi:dәn/ ‘they avoided.’ Nor is the phenomenon
restricted to these particular consonants; one also finds the alternation /-k/ d /-g-/ contrasting with
invariant /k/(/verk/ ‘work,’ pl. /verkә/ but /t s verk/ ‘dwarf,’ pl. /t s vergә/, etc.), the alternation /-s/ d /- z -/
contrasting with invariant /s/, and so on.
Because this phenomenon occurs in the inflection of words of different morphological classes, its origin
cannot plausibly be attributed to morphological change; after all, it is most unlikely that three or more
different morphological changes would give precisely the same result. Because a large proportion of the
language's basic vocabulary is involved, any explanation involving borrowing from another language 4 is
likewise implausible. Only sound change could reasonably have given rise to so pervasive a pattern, and the
suspicion that sound change is responsible is confirmed by the details of the pattern: it involves a natural
class of sounds, namely obstruents, in a clearly definable phonotactic position, namely at the ends of
phonological words.
Once it is clear that sound change is responsible for the observed pattern, we can exploit the fact that sound
change is overwhelmingly regular – that is, that the conditions which govern sound change are strictly
phonological. If the stem-final consonant had originally been *t in all the forms adduced above and had
become /-d-/ between vowels, we would be unable to explain why it had become /-d-/ in /p f a:dә/ ‘paths'
but not in /gra:tә/ ‘ridges,’ and so on, since no phonological conditioning for the difference can be stated.
Therefore we must conclude that the paradigms in question were originally 5 *ta:t, *ta:tәn; *p f a:d, *p f a:dә;
*gra:d, *gra:dә (‘degree’); *gra:t, *gra:tә (‘ridge’); and so on, and that the alternation between word-final /-
t/ and non-final /-d-/ was created by a regular sound change which devoiced word-final obstruents
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g97814051274795
Page 2 of 14
665559040.009.png
3. Internal Reconstruction : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:29 PM
t/ and non-final /-d-/ was created by a regular sound change which devoiced word-final obstruents
(affecting also *-g, *-z, etc.; see above). The exceptionlessness of sound change is reflected in the
exceptionlessness of the alternation, which is completely automatic: one simply does not find word-final /-
d/, etc., in this variety of German.
Since this is a maximally simple example with which further examples will be compared, it is worth noting a
number of additional facts about it. Because the alternation between voiced and voiceless obstruents is fully
automatic, it remains fully transparent to the native speaker: a theory of phonology which permits any
abstraction from surface contrasts at all will analyze the alternation /-t/ d /-d-/ simply as /d/, and in fact
that is the analysis reflected in Standard German spelling ( Pfad, Pfade , etc.). In such a simple case IR
replicates phonological analysis point for point, and the reconstruction of the earlier state of affairs is
achieved simply by deleting a single phonological rule from the grammar.
Yet even in such a straightforward example, not every detail of our reconstruction is historically accurate.
For instance, IR fails to tell us that Grad ‘degree,’ unlike the other nouns listed above, was borrowed into
German well after the devoicing of word-final obstruents occurred. 6 That Grad should nevertheless exhibit
the alternation is scarcely surprising: once devoicing of word-final obstruents had become an exceptionless,
“surfacey” phonological rule, every new loanword ending in a voiced obstruent would have become subject
to it automatically. But it should be clear that the chronological relationship between the acquisition of new
lexemes and the acquisition of postlexical phonological rules will not, in general, be recoverable by IR, since
it is the nature of such rules to apply to any and all lexemes regardless of their origins.
Another detail which IR cannot recover is the original identity of non-alternating phonemes in the position of
neutralization; for example, IR will not tell us whether the final stop of the invariant particle /unt/ ‘and' was
originally *t or *d. A related problem involves lexemes that ought to exhibit the alternation but are seldom
used in the form(s) in which the neutralization failed to occur. For example, the Old High German noun
‘value, worth’ and adjective ‘valuable, worth’ were both werd , and one might expect that the modern words
would be “/vert/ d /verd-/,” with underlying //d//; but in fact we find invariant /vert/, /vert-/, with
underlying // t //. Apparently the unsuffixed form, which was affected by the regular devoicing of word-final
obstruents, was so much commoner (or more salient) than all the inflected forms together that its surface
/-t/ was reanalyzed as underlying /t/ by some past generation of German speakers. Since the result is a
non-alternating paradigm, IR cannot recover this sequence of events; instead we are led to reconstruct a
historically inaccurate *vert, *vert-.
This last type of development is among those traditionally called “analogical changes” – in effect, changes
that depend (at least in part) on morphological structure, 7 as opposed to sound changes, which are strictly
phonetic or (low-level) phonological. Both types of change, occurring subsequently to a given sound change
that gave rise to a given alternation, can increase the difficulty of IR from that alternation in a variety of
ways. Two cases that illustrate these processes are the fronting of *a: in the Attic dialect of Ancient Greek
and the rhotacism of intervocalic *s in early Latin.
The historical changes that affected *a: in Attic Greek were not simple (see Szemerényi 1968; Gates 1976),
but the net result of the changes was a straightforward alternation: original *a: appears as /ⓘ:/ (merging
with original /ⓘ:/) except when preceded by /i/, / e /, or /r/, in which positions it remains as /a:/. The
distribution of /a:/ and /ⓘ:/ is quite clear, and the alternation between them is pervasive in Attic Greek
morphology; it appears in the singular endings of hundreds of “first declension” nouns and adjectives, in the
sigmatic aorists of verbs with roots ending in resonants (Smyth 1956: 173), in a small class of very common
verb stems (“mi-verbs”; Smyth 1956: 134–9), and so on. This wide distribution makes it clear that the
alternation is the result of a sound change. Both /ⓘ:/ and /a:/ appear without restriction after front vowels
and /r/, but examples of /a:/ not after a front vowel or /r/ are usually explainable as more recent
developments (see below); the principle that sound changes are regular therefore leads us to reconstruct this
change as “*a: > /ⓘ:/ except after /i, e, r/.” 8 The merger of *a: and *ⓘ: occurred in so many phonological
environments that it is sometimes not clear from internal evidence which older sound a given instance of /
ⓘ:/ reflects simply because it does not happen to occur after /i/, /e/, or /r/. That *a: was the original sound
is sometimes shown by the fact that /ⓘ:/ alternates with short /a/ (the latter appearing, for example, in the
plural endings of first declension nouns and adjectives), while original /ⓘ:/ alternates with short /e/.
Compare the following partial paradigms of some Attic Greek verbs:
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g97814051274795
Page 3 of 14
665559040.010.png
3. Internal Reconstruction : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:29 PM
/apédra:/
/éstⓘ:/
/ésbⓘ:/
‘(s)he ran away’
‘(s)he stood up’
‘it [the fire] went out’
/apodrâ:nai/
/st :nai/
/sb :nai/
‘to run away’
‘to stand up’
‘to be extinguished’
/apodraíⓘ:/
/staíⓘ:/
/sbeíⓘ:/
let him/her run away’
‘let him/her stand up’
‘let it go out’
/apodrántes/
/stântes/
/sbéntes/ ‘(upon)
‘(upon) running away
(nom.pl.masc.)’
‘(upon) standing up
(nom.pl.masc.)’
being extinguished
(nom.pl.masc.)’
Note that in the third and fourth form given for each verb the vowel after the /r/, /t/, or /b/ respectively
appears shortened, and ‘stand up’ shows /a/, like ‘run away’ but unlike ‘be extinguished’ – showing that the
/ⓘ:/ of /éstⓘ:/ and /st :nai/ was originally *a:.
This clear pattern has been complicated by a considerable number of subsequent changes, but not all have
been equally disruptive. Many new /a:/'s have arisen by two later sound changes, vowel contraction and the
“second compensatory lengthening” (2CL); however, those changes also gave rise to alternations from which
the original state of affairs is still recoverable, and for that reason they do not seriously obscure the /a:/ d /
ⓘ:/ alternation. For example, compare forms of the present tense of /tolmâ:n/ ‘dare’ with the corresponding
forms of /p h ére:n/ ‘be carrying’ :
/tolmô:men/ ‘we dare’ /péromen/ ‘we are carrying’
/tolmâ:te/ ‘you (pl.) dare’ /p h érete/ ‘you (pl.) are carrying’
The paradigms differ in two ways. In the first place, if we consider the vowels between the invariant root-
syllables /tolm-/, /p h er-/ and the invariant endings /-men/, /-te/, it is clear that the vowels found in
‘dare’ are both longer and lower than the corresponding vowels of ‘be carrying’, but that they resemble the
latter to some extent (‘we …’ always shows a back round vowel, for example). Second, the accent falls on
the third syllable from the end of the word in ‘be carrying,’ but on the second syllable from the end in ‘dare.’
When we consider also the fact that there is a noun /tólma/ ‘courage’ obviously related to ‘dare,’ it becomes
clear that the most plausible and economical way to account for all these phenomena is to posit earlier
forms *tolmáomen for /tolm :men/ and *tolmáete for /tolmâ:te/. The /a:/ of the latter form, then, resulted
from contraction of the sequence *ae; and because we can explain its appearance by such a development, it
does not seriously obscure the pattern according to which we expect /a:/ after /i, e, r/ but /ⓘ:/ elsewhere –
a pattern which, we now see, applies only to those older (“original”) instances of *a: which existed before
vowel contraction had occurred. In fact, the larger pattern now permits us to reconstruct the relative
chronology of the sound changes involved: the change “*a: > /ⓘ:/ except after /i, e, r/” must have run its
course before the change “*ae > /a:/” produced new /a:/s, since those new /a:/'s did not undergo the
former change. 9
The 2CL can likewise be recovered from the patterns of alternation to which it gave rise. Consider the
following partial noun and adjective paradigms:
‘guard’
‘serf’
‘black’
‘(upon) standing up’
nom.sg.masc.
/p h ülaks/ /t h
:s/
/méla:s/ /stá:s/
nom.pl.masc.
/p h ülakes/ /t h
:tes/
/mélanes/ /stántes/
The invariant endings are nom.sg. /-s/ and nom.pl. /-es/, and the stem of ‘guard’ is likewise invariant
/p h ülak-/; but the other stems participate in various alternations. The stem of ‘serf’ appears as / t h
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g97814051274795
Page 4 of 14
665559040.001.png 665559040.002.png 665559040.003.png
3. Internal Reconstruction : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:29 PM
/p h ülak-/; but the other stems participate in various alternations. The stem of ‘serf’ appears as / t h : t -/ 10
when a vowel follows, but as / t h :-/ when followed by /- s /; and since fuller study of the grammar shows
that /- t -/ was not normally inserted between vowels in Greek, we must conclude that the nom.sg. was
originally *t h :ts, and that stem-final *-t- was lost when *-s followed immediately. By a similar line of
reasoning we conclude that the nom.sg.masc. of ‘black’ was originally *mélans, and that the sequence *ans
became /a:s/ by the 2CL. Finally, in the nom.sg.masc. of the participle of ‘stand up’ both changes have
occurred – first the loss of *-t-, then the 2CL – and the development can be reconstructed as *stánts >
*stáns > /stá:s/. This accounts for numerous additional cases of unexpected /a:/; and of course the fronting
of *a: must likewise have run its course before the 2CL occurred.
One would expect greater disruption to have resulted from changes that tended to obscure the /a:/ d /ⓘ:/
alternation and are not reconstructible . For example, already in the sixth century BCE, Attic possessed two
noun stems which obviously belong in the first declension 11 but show stem-final /ⓘ:/ < *a: after /r/,
/kórⓘ:/ ‘girL' and /dérⓘ:/ ‘necklace.’ Comparative evidence from other dialects shows that these words
originally had a *w before the stem-final vowel (cf. Arkadian <korwa> 12 ‘Persephone’, <derwa> ‘ridge, spur
(of a hill)’), and it is reasonable to infer that the *w had not yet been lost in Attic when the fronting of *a:
occurred (so that the *a: was not then immediately preceded by *r); but since *w was subsequently lost
without a trace in Attic, IR cannot recover those events. A similar case is /kórrⓘ:/ ‘temple (of the head)’ :
comparative evidence shows that /kórrⓘ:/ < *kórsⓘ: (preserved unchanged in East Ionic) < *kórsa: (preserved
unchanged in East Aiolic), but that development could not be recovered from Attic evidence alone. Still other
cases of the same sort are Attic /póa:/ ‘grass’ < *poía: and /stoâ:/ ‘colonnade’ < *stoiá: (both preserved
unchanged in Doric dialects). At least one analogical change contributes further examples. A coherent class
of pairs of present and aorist stems show the expected pattern pres. /-aíne:n/, aor. /-ǎ:nai/ d /- :nai/, the
vowel alternation in the aorist stem depending on the preceding sound:
Present
Aorist
/hügiaíne:n/ ‘be welL’ /hügiâ:nai/ ‘get welL’
/ksⓘ:raíne:n/ ‘be drying (it) out’ /ksⓘ:râ:nai/ ‘dry (it) out’
/p h aíne:n/ ‘show (continually)’ /p h :nai/ ‘show’
/sⓘ:maíne:n/ ‘indicate (continually)’ /sⓘ:m :nai/ ‘indicate’
/ksaíne:n/ ‘scratch (repeatedly)’
/ksⓘ:nai/ ‘scratch (once)’
/k h alepaíne:n/ ‘be offended’
/k h alep :nai/ ‘take offense’
and so on (the list could be extended considerably). But toward the end of the fifth century BCE we find a
few aorists with /a:/ not after /i, e, r/:
Present
Aorist
/koilaíne:n/ ‘be hollowing
out’
/koilâ:nai/
‘hollow out' (Thucydides 100.4.2)
/kerdaíne:n/ ‘gain’
/kerdâ:nai/ ‘make a profit’ (Andocides 1.134; 13 Xenophon, Apology of
Socrates 9)
The /a:/ of these aorists can only be the result of analogy with other aorists in which /a:/ is etymologically
justified (though exactly which verbs provided the model for the analogical change is not clear).
But all these exceptions together do not suffice to obscure the pattern from which the fronting of original
long *a: can be reconstructed internally, for a simple reason: there are hundreds of forms which show the
expected alternation, and very few which fail to show it. In most cases IR can only identify these exceptional
forms, not explain why they fail to behave as expected; and it should also be obvious that IR cannot tell us
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g97814051274795
Page 5 of 14
665559040.004.png 665559040.005.png 665559040.006.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin