Taber, Redden - Functional Anatomy of H. - Positive Affect.pdf

(13099 KB) Pobierz
untitled
WINDOWS TO THE BRAIN
Robin A. Hurley, M.D., L. Anne Hayman, M.D., Katherine H. Taber, Ph.D.
Section Editors
Functional Anatomy of Humor: Positive Affect and Chronic Mental Illness
Katherine H. Taber, Ph.D., Maurice Redden, M.D., Robin A. Hurley, M.D.
COVER AND FIGURE 1. The dopamine system is a critical component of the brain reward circuitry, important for both motivation
and reward-associated functions. The majority of dopamine-containing neurons reside in two midbrain nuclei, the substantia nigra (pink)
and the ventral tegmental area (yellow). The dorsal striatum (caudate and putamen) is the major target for the substantia nigra. The
ventral striatum (blue, mostly occupied by nucleus accumbens) is a major target for the ventral tegmental area.
FIGURE 2. Functional imaging agrees
with clinical evidence that the neuroana-
tomical networks supporting voluntary
and humor-evoked smiling are different.
Motor-related areas (e.g. primary motor
cortex, premotor cortex, operculum) are
consistently activated during voluntary
facial movement (left), but not during
humor-evoked smiling (right). Differ-
ences in the areas activated probably re-
late to the different contrasts utilized in
these three studies. 1–3
FIGURE 3. One approach to identifying areas of brain important for later stages of humor processing is to look for areas of activation
in common between two different types of humor (conjunction analysis). 4,5 A study comparing verbally-presented puns and semantic
jokes reported that only an area in medial prefrontal cortex (blue) survived the two step analysis procedure. A study comparing visually-
presented captioned and uncaptioned cartoons reported multiple regions of activation in common (green).
http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 19:4, Fall 2007
732213337.002.png
TABER et al.
sively studied, investigation of positive emotions
is relatively new. 6 An evolutionary perspective on the
value of emotions is potentially illuminating. The adap-
tive value of many negative emotions has been related
to promoting fast and appropriate responding during
high risk situations (fight or flight). 7 Under life-threat-
ening circumstances, a narrowing of response patterns
is necessary for rapid response selection and perfor-
mance. In contrast it has been proposed that the adap-
tive value of humor and positive affect is to promote a
broadening of response. This provides a way to explore
new possibilities in times of safety. 7 In Fredrickson’s
model, general categories of positive emotions have dif-
ferent but complementary adaptive value that build an
individual’s physical, intellectual and social resources.
The emotion of joy (or happiness), for example, is asso-
ciated with imaginative play, which is characterized by
“exploration, invention, and just plain fooling around.” 7
Among other benefits, play promotes the learning of new
skills. The emotion of interest (or wonder) promotes ex-
ploration, leading to a broadening and deepening of
knowledge.
The adaptive value of positive emotions is supported
by their evolutionarily early origins. Both spontaneous
smiling and laughing have been documented in non-
human primate species, most commonly as part of social
play. 8–10 Gervais and Wilson proposed “nonserious so-
cial incongruity” as the common factor for the stimuli
that elicit spontaneous laughter, which include safe sur-
prise (infants), tickling and physical play (apes, human
children), and incongruity-based humor (human adults,
signing apes). 10 The authors note that laughter is both a
behavioral response and a signal. In their formulation,
laughter and humor benefit both the sender and receiver
by spreading positive emotions that promote stability,
decrease negativity, moderate stress, and strengthen
group identity and cohesion.
Positive emotions presumably activate areas of the
brain involved in reward. 11 The dopamine system is a
critical component of the brain reward circuitry, im-
portant for motivation, affect and reward-associated
functions (Figure 1). 12–16 The majority of dopamine-
containing neurons reside in two midbrain nuclei, the
substantia nigra and the ventral tegmental area (VTA).
The dorsal striatum (caudate and putamen) is the ma-
jor target for the substantia nigra (nigro-striatal projec-
tion). The ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) is a
major target for the VTA. The VTA projects to multiple
areas of both the cerebral cortex (mesocortical projec-
tions) and limbic system (mesolimbic projections).
It is important to distinguish true humor-evoked
laughter and smiling (Duchenne), which are spontane-
ous emotionally positive responses, from laughter and
smiling that are intentionally generated (non Du-
chenne). 8,10 The neurological pathways involved are
clearly different, as indicated by lesion-induced paresis
of voluntary facial expression with preservation of emo-
tionally evoked facial expression and vice versa (double-
dissociation). 8 Volitional facial paresis has been associ-
ated with lesions in primary motor cortex, premotor
areas (including the frontal operculum), and/or along
the course of the corticobulbar motor tracts. Emotional
paresis has been reported in patients with Parkinson’s
disease, and has been associated with lesions in the thal-
amus and striatal structures as well as in the pons (i.e.,
vicinity of the facial nucleus, CN VII). Recent functional
imaging studies have further delineated the cortical ar-
eas involved (Figure 2). 1–3 Activations (as measured by
either positron emission tomography [PET] or func-
tional MRI [fMRI]) were found within primary motor
cortex, supplementary motor cortex, and the frontal
operculum during voluntary smiling. Primary motor ar-
eas were not activated by humor-induced smiling.
Humor is a complex construct, with several stages of
processing. Although multiple theories of humor exist,
the most accepted is the Incongruity Theory postulated
by Sols. 8,17 He proposed that humor occurs in two
stages. The first involves the listener’s expectation of the
joke being disconfirmed by the end; the listener encoun-
ters an incongruity (i.e., the punch-line). In the second
stage the listener engages in a form of problem solving
to resolve the incongruity between the punch-line and
the expectation shaped by the joke. This has also been
described as “surprise” and “coherence,” a similar for-
mulation to that of “nonserious social incongruity” (see
above). A third stage has been proposed: detecting that
From the Veterans Affairs Mid Atlantic Mental Illness Research, Edu-
cation, and Clinical Center (K.H.T., R.A.H.); the Mental Health Ser-
vice Line, Salisbury Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Salisbury, NC
(K.H.T., R.A.H.); the Division of Biomedical Sciences, Virginia School
of Osteopathic Medicine, Blacksburg, VA (K.H.T.); the Department
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Baylor College of Medicine,
Houston, TX (K.H.T); Department of Psychiatry, Wake Forest Uni-
versity School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC (M.R.., R.A.H.); De-
partment of Radiology, Wake Forest University School of Medicine,
Winston-Salem, NC (R.A.H.); and the Menninger Department of Psy-
chiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Baylor College of Medicine, Hous-
ton, TX (R.A.H.). Address correspondence to Dr. Robin Hurley, Hef-
ner VA Medical Center, 1601 Brenner Ave., Salisbury, NC 28144;
Robin.Hurley@med.va.gov (e-mail).
Copyright
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 19:4, Fall 2007
http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org
359
W hile negative emotional states have been exten-
2007 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.
732213337.003.png
FUNCTIONAL ANATOMY OF HUMOR
what actually makes sense is pleasant nonsense (appre-
ciation). 8
Functional imaging provides a way to identify areas
of brain involved in different types of humor and stages
of processing. A common approach has been to look for
areas in which activation correlates with a measure of
amusement intensity (funniness). 1–5,18,19 Results can be
complex to interpret. Such activations may be due mul-
tiple processes including solving the incongruity in or-
der to understand the joke and appreciation of the hu-
mor. Results may also be influenced by how the degree
of funniness is determined. For example, a study that
used the intensity of smiling (as measured by electro-
myography) found activity correlated with smiling only
in medial prefrontal cortex (BA 6) and putamen, both
motor-related areas. 1 Studies that have used subject rat-
ings of funniness, on the other hand, have generally
found much more widespread activations.
Two studies have used conjunction analysis in order
to identify areas of common activation between two
types of humor (Figure 3). 4,5 One study reported a single
area (medial prefrontal cortex, BA 10/11) survived the
two step analysis procedure when comparing two types
of verbally presented humor (puns and semantic jokes). 4
The other study compared two types of visually pre-
sented humor (captioned and uncaptioned cartoons). 5
Multiple regions of activation were found, including
posterior temporal cortex, inferior frontal cortex, hip-
pocampus and parahippocampal cortex, amygdala, nu-
cleus accumbens and midbrain.
Given the adaptive value of humor in promoting posi-
tive emotions, the brain areas activated during the final
stage (appreciation) are of particular interest. 20 One in-
triguing study has used event-related fMRI during pre-
sentation of humorous films (with and without a laugh
track) to compare the areas activated just prior to laugh-
ter (humor detection) to those active during laughter
(humor appreciation). 21 Humor detection was associ-
ated with activation in the inferior frontal and posterior
temporal cortices for both types of film. Humor appre-
ciation was associated with activation in the insular cor-
tex and amygdala. These results are consistent with pre-
vious studies, and support the importance of the
posterior temporal and inferior frontal regions in resolv-
ing the incongruity. The authors of the study also noted
that activation of insular cortex and amygdala is consis-
tent with the visceral and emotional aspects of humor
appreciation. The involvement of the amygdala may
also relate to its role in memory formation, as humorous
material is more easily remembered than material that
is not.
Although the beneficial effects of humor and laughter
have been part of “common wisdom” for many centu-
ries, there is comparatively little solid scientific research
in this potentially quite important area. 22,23 As noted
above, humor is a complex construct. In research studies
humor has been defined in many ways including a stim-
ulus (e.g., viewing a cartoon or humorous film), a re-
sponse (e.g., laughter), a personality trait (e.g., positive
affect), a cognitive process (e.g., resolving the contradic-
tion to understand a joke) and a therapeutic intervention
(e.g., humor therapy). The time frame examined has var-
ied from a few minutes to years, depending upon the
aspect of humor under investigation. The group sizes
range from fewer than 10 individuals to cohorts num-
bering in the thousands.
Studies that focus on the physiological effects of
laughter suggest positive effects on many aspects in-
cluding immunity and pain tolerance, although some
have been criticized on methodological grounds. 22 A
more recent review using the broader definition of posi-
tive affect (e.g., feelings that reflect a level of pleasurable
engagement with the environment) evaluated studies of
physical health. 23 The authors differentiated studies of
trait positive affect (a stable disposition) and state posi-
tive affect (short-term bout), noting that the former
would be more likely to influence chronic disease pro-
cesses, while the latter would be more likely to influ-
ence occurrence of sudden events. They found that
both prospective and cross-sectional studies support
an association between trait positive affect and better
health, with lower morbidity in a wide range of con-
ditions (e.g., stroke, accident, common cold). This did
not hold for all diseases, particularly those with a high
short-term mortality rate (e.g., melanoma). Laboratory
studies utilizing induction of state positive affect dem-
onstrated generally positive results (e.g., decreased
pain sensitivity/increased pain tolerance, improved
immune response). Laboratory studies of the effect on
stress responses (as indicated by cortisol level) were less
consistent in showing a positive effect. The authors of
this review emphasized that there are multiple ways in
which positive affect might influence health, some direct
(e.g., changes in health practices) and some indirect
(e.g., susceptibility to disease, sensitivity to stress).
Humor has been integrated into the practice of med-
icine in multiple ways. 24–26 Perhaps the most common
use of humor is to decrease the sense of isolation that is
360
http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 19:4, Fall 2007
732213337.004.png
TABER et al.
often felt by patients, families, and health care workers.
Appropriate use of humor can promote communication
and strengthen relationships. Humor must be used with
care, however, as poorly timed or ill-chosen humor can
be detrimental. Clowns have been used in medicine for
about two decades. A recent study compared anxiety
levels between children accompanied only by parents
and children accompanied by both parents and clowns
during the preoperative period prior to minor surgery. 27
The presence of clowns was associated with signifi-
cantly lower preoperative anxiety, supporting a thera-
peutic benefit. Interestingly, surveys completed by the
medical staff indicated both perception of the benefit to
the child and widespread opposition to continuing the
program.
Two recent studies have explored the effect of very
different humor-centered activities on the behavior of
hospitalized psychiatric patients. 28,29 In one, twice-
weekly clown-lead sessions were provided on an acute
psychiatric ward. 28 Sessions were varied, and included
games, psychomotor expression exercises and activities
based on imaginary situations. Disruptive behaviors
during the intervention period were compared to the
same period of time (83 days) prior to the intervention.
Although there were more patients admitted during in-
tervention period, (101 versus 83) disruptive behaviors
decreased in both absolute and relative terms. Of par-
ticular interest, the most significantly reduced behaviors
were attempted elopements, self-injury and fighting.
This change is not likely to be due to differences in the
patient populations, as the distribution by diagnosis
was quite similar during the two time periods. The au-
thors of the study note that most of the treatment plan-
ning in psychiatry is directed toward psychotherapeutic
and pharmacotherapeutic inteventions. There has been
relatively little focus on the therapeutic milieu. In the
second study, movies were provided on two chronic
psychiatric wards 5 days per week for 3 months. 29 One
group viewed only humorous films. The other group
viewed a mixture of film types, with only 15% being
humorous. The patients were primarily diagnosed with
chronic schizophrenia. A significant reduction in clini-
cally rated negative symptoms, anxiety, and depression
was found only in the group that viewed humorous
movies. In addition, self-reported anger was decreased
and social competence was improved. No changes were
found in positive symptoms, activities of daily living,
treatment insight, or therapeutic alliance. The authors of
the study noted that humorous movies appeared to gen-
erate active social exchanges not seen following the non-
humorous (e.g., action adventure) movies. Although
these studies must be considered preliminary, they both
suggest that humor-based interventions can be quite
beneficial to patients with chronic mental illness.
Recently, there has been an increasing emphasis on
recovery-oriented care in the treatment of chronic men-
tal illness. A review of research in this area identified
multiple mechanisms by which pleasure, play and posi-
tive life events help to develop and strengthen a pa-
tient’s ability to take an active role in promoting recov-
ery. 30 These insights support the need for further study
of the functional anatomy of positive emotions.
References
1. Iwase M, Ouchi Y, Okada H, Yokoyama, et al: Neural substrates
of human facial expression of pleasant emotion induced by
comic films: a PET study. Neuroimage 2002; 17:758–768
2. Wild B, Erb M, Eyb M, et al: Why are smiles contagious? An
fMRI study of the interaction between perception of facial effect
and facial movements. Psychiatry Res 2003; 123:17–36
3. Wild B, Rodden FA, Rapp A, et al: Humor and smiling: cortical
regions selective for cognitive, affective, and volitional compo-
nents. Neurology 2006; 66:887–893
4. Goel V, Dolan RJ: The functional anatomy of humor: segregating
cognitive and affective componenets. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4:237–
238
5. Watson KK, Matthews BJ, Allman JM: Brain activation during
sight gags and language-dependent humor. Cereb Cortex 2007;
17:314–324
6. Burgdorf J, Panksepp J: The neurobiology of positive emotions.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006; 30:173–187
7. Fredrickson BL: What good are positive emotions? Rev Gen Psy-
chol 1998; 2:300–319
8. Wild B, Rodden FA, GroddW, et al: Neural correlates of laughter
and humour. Brain 2003; 126:2121–2138
9. Pearce JMS: Some neurological aspects of laughter. Eur Neurol
2004; 52:169–171
10. Gervais M , Wilson DS: The evolution and functions of laughter
and humor: a synthetic approach. Q Rev Biol 2005; 80:395–430
11. Esch T , Stefano GB: The neurobiology of pleasure, reward pro-
cesses, addiction and their health implications. Neuro Endocri-
nol Lett 2004; 25:235–251
12. Schultz W: Muultiple reward signals in the brain. Nat Rev Neu-
rosci 2000; 1:199–207
13. Wise RA: Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensed incen-
tives. Neuron 2002; 36:229–240
14. Delgado MR: Reward-related responses in the human striatum.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007; 1104:70–88
15. Berridge KC: The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the
case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;
191:391–431
16. Nicola SM: The nucleus accumbens as part of a basal ganglia
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 19:4, Fall 2007
http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org
361
732213337.005.png
FUNCTIONAL ANATOMY OF HUMOR
action selection ciruit. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007; 191:
521–550
17. Suls JA: A two-stage model for the appreciation of jokes and
cartoons. in The psychology of humor: theoretical perspectives
adn empirical issues, Edited by Goldstein JH , McGhee P. New
York, Academic Press 1972, pp 81–100
18. Mobbs D, Greicius MD, Abdel-Azim E, et al: Humor modulates
the mesolimbic reward centers. Neuron 2003; 40:1041–1048
19. Goldin PR, Hutcherson CA, Ochsner KN, et al: The neural bases
of amusement and sadness: a comparison of block contrast and
subject-specif emotion intensity regression approaches. Neu-
roimage 2005; 27:26–36
20. Berns GS: Something funny happened to reward. Trends Cogn
Sci 2004; 8:193–194
21. Moran JM, Wig GS, Adams RB, et al: Neural correlates of humor
detection and appreciation. Neuroimage 2004; 21:1055–1060
22. Martin RA: Humor, laughter, and physical health: methodolog-
ical issues and research findings. Psychol Bull 2001; 127:504–519
23. Pressman SD, Cohen,S: Does positive affect influence health?
Psychol Bull 2005; 131:925–971
24. Rosner F: Therapeutic efficacy of laughter in medicine. Cancer
Invest 2002; 20:434–436
25. Dziegielewski SF, Jacinto GA, Laudadio A, et al: An essential
communication tool in therapy. International Journal of Mental
Health 2003; 32:74–90
26. Penson RT, Partridge,RA, Rudd,P, et al: Laughter: the best med-
icine? Oncologist 2005; 10:651–660
27. Vagnoli L, Caprilli S, Robiglio A, et al: Clown doctors as a treat-
ment for preoperative anxiety in children: A randomized, pro-
spective study. Pediatrics 2005; 116:e563–e567
28. Higueras A, Carretero-Dios H, Munoz JP, et al: Effects of a hu-
mor-centered activity on disruptive behavior in patients in a
general hospital psychiatric ward. Int J ClinHealth Psychol 2006;
6:53–64
29. Gelkopf M, Gonen B, Kurs R, et al: The effect of humorous mov-
ies on inpatients with chronic schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis
2006; 194:880–883
30. Davidson L, Shahar G, Lawless MS, et al: Play, pleasure, and
other positive life events: “non-specific” factors in recovery from
mental illness? Psychiatry 2006; 69:151–163
362
http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 19:4, Fall 2007
732213337.001.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin